In this post, I will attempt to critically think about advices and how to deliver them.
I have recently been thinking about advices as I realized how detrimental / harmful they could be, how inefficient they may sometimes be, and how to tackle these unfortunates outcomes. This is applicable to social media, in personal encounters, or in health-care relationships (and probably other types of interactions but these are the ones I have been able to relate this topic to at the moment).
Providing an advice is always an act of kindness, a benevolent manifestation of caring … but it may not be perceived as such by the person receiving it. After all, aren’t we all experiencing in the world subjectively according to our lives and context? Let me illustrate this with examples.
I) A patient tells me they tried to start physical activity and started running everyday. As a healthcare provider, I am concerned about their safety and want to advise them to be careful about overload as they were sedentary for years. I want to prevent potential injuries. My first reaction is to want to say: “Great, but you should be careful, if you run everyday you may get injured”.
II) I see a video of somebody on social media doing an exercise and realize they do not perform the movement with optimal pattern. I want to help them achieving the perfect form, to avoid them wasting their time and help them improve. Then, I am keen on commenting below the post: “Your form is not perfect, you should try to work on x or y, or try to change this parameter as it will help you”.
III) A friend tells me they have decided to go vegan, and I worry about their health which would led me to say: “Cool, but you should take B12 vitamin to avoid issues”.
In scenario I), it is easy to think it is my role to provide this advice for the person’s safety, and act preventively. Yet, the person’s may receive it in a different way. Maybe it will reduce their initial motivation to do physical activity. Maybe they will think it is unsafe for them to run because I (their health-care provider), am concerned about their running. Maybe they will get offended that I do not believe in their ability to self manage. Any situation could happen here.
In scenario II) I am on social media, so I comment my advice thinking I will help the person doing better. But maybe this person received several comments like mine. Maybe they will feel criticized. Maybe they will feel the hard-work does not pay and they are not good enough. Maybe they will feel they should stop trying.
In scenario III), even though I just wanted to take care of a friend, maybe their experience was different. Maybe they will think I consider myself more knowledgable than them on a topic they love. Maybe they know more then me about it. Maybe they will feel isolated and misunderstood in their project. Maybe they will feel angry at me doubting them. Maybe they will feel hopeless for the future of planet and animal well-being and depressed by society and humankind.
In conclusion, either my advice was detrimental (decreasing their confidence, hopes, or creating anger), or inefficient / useless (as the person will not listen).
2. To tackle these situations, here are some ideas I may want to consider prior to say or write my advice.
A) I shall make sure the advice is embedded in a chat. I could start a conversation first. When someone comes to tell me I should do something without saying hello, I may legitimately feel irritated. So, I can start talking first and engage with the person. Example II) : before telling my friend the exercise they posted on social media is not perfect, I can get news about them. “I saw your video on social media, it made me realise I wanted to get new from you, how are you doing? :)”
B) Then, once the conversation is started, I can begin by congratulating the person for what they are doing. Compliments and encouragements are always welcome. Example I): “Congratulation for starting running, it is a great effort you are doing here, this is inspiring!”.
C) I may want to genuinely ask for prior knowledge. Most of the time I do not know how much the person already knows about the advice I want to deliver. And if they already know about it, what is the point of my advice? Example III): “I heard many things about vegan diet, are you aware of potential issues associated with it?”
D) If they they are aware and have the same understanding as me, then I can be reassured and no need to go further. But if we have a different view on the topic, I may want to ask if they would like to know my perception (ask for permission). This allows me to make sure: -They are willing to know more / -They are in the mental disposition to hear my perspective / advice = they are available for it (sometimes it is just not the right time) / -They will listen leading to better chances of my advice to be helpful. Example II): “It seems that I have a different understanding about how to perform this exercise, would you like me to share what I know about it with you?”
E) Then I can provide my advice in a kind and appropriate manner, choosing my words carefully. Example I): “In my experience as a physiotherapist, I have observed that overload may happen when people tend to go back to physical activity. Some even get injured when starting with high frequency and intensity due to their motivation. What can be helpful is to monitor symptoms this way, and if needed reduce the frequency to x times a week, and after a while gradually increase it”.
F) Bonus: I can even go further by asking for their thoughts about it. It allows me to see if my message was understood, how they felt about it and maybe identify other topics of discussion. Example III): “What do you think about it? Does it resonate with your experience?”
Here were the key elements I identified that could help providing advices more effectively in professional practice and life. Some elements may be similar in motivational interviewing or other types of communication strategies.
I am open to hear about how you do provide advices and your thoughts about this topic so let me know them in the comment section.
Do spinal mobilization / manipulation techniques have
a role in treatment of patients with LBP?
When I first trained to graduate as a physiotherapist,
I learned techniques to mobilize joints of the back. At this point of my life I
had absolutely no idea of any form of critical thinking. When I began my MSc, I
discovered that physiotherapy existed in social media. I started to witness a
debate opposing those for and those against manual therapy. I could not know
who was right, so I probably decided to believe the group that created the less
“dissonance” with what I learned, and the postgraduate courses I paid for. Then
during my MSc postgraduate training, I have been asked to explain why I was
using such techniques. This question made me stop and realize that I did not
know why I used them. I just did it because I was taught to and because I read
experts physios on social media that seemed to encourage it. With some step
back and information about cognitive bias, I then reached the following
conclusion. I did not know why I used these techniques, and my relationship
with manual therapy belonged more to faith than science. Now that I learned to
think, research and critique information, I will attempt to give some insights
on the effects of spinal mobilizations and manipulations techniques in the
field of manual therapy, hoping that it will encourage readers to reflect on
their own practice too. The techniques discussed in my blog will be the passive
accessory intervertebral movements (PAIVMS), passive physiological
intervertebral vertebral movements (PPIVMS), the mobilization with movement
(MWM) called sustained natural apophyseal glide (SNAG) when applied to the
spine and grade 5 manipulation techniques. I will use the “spinal mobilization
and manipulation therapy” SMMT acronym to encompass the aforementioned
techniques.
Why do spinal
mobilization and manipulation techniques work? What are the current hypotheses
regarding their effects?
When I reviewed literature, I found that the
mechanisms underlying SMMT are not yet fully understood. However, three main
mechanisms are used to discuss their effects. The mechanical effects, the
neurophysiological effects and the non-specific effects. Each of these will be
discussed in a separate paragraph through the blog.
Mechanical
effects.
The mechanical effects are all effects that can be
produced only by mechanical and biomechanical properties of tissues affected by
the technique. One of the first hypothesis regarding the manual therapy’s
techniques was that they could “detect and correct biomechanical faults” (Joel E. Bialosky, George, & Bishop, 2008). It also
was first hypothesis of Brian Mulligan to explain the good outcomes he had with
his MWM as he proposed the “positional fault hypothesis” (PFH) in 1989. These similar
hypotheses seem not supported by evidence for several reasons. First because
the existence of the “faults” has not yet been demonstrated. Measuring them
validly and reliably remains difficult even when using powerful imagery
techniques, although it was studied in peripheral superficial joints (Vicenzino, Hing, Rivett, & Hall, 2011). This is
all the more complicated for clinicians and regarding the lumbar spine’s joints
that are not directly accessible to visual inspection and manual palpation. Secondly,
in lumbar spine, clinicians seem unable to agree on the location of the “fault”
(Seffinger et al., 2004). In this
systematic review the research team noticed that the majority of spinal
palpatory diagnostic procedures are not reliable and regarding lumbar spine,
that regional is superior to segmental range of motion assessment. In other
words, we could say that it is not possible to identify reliably a segmental
“fault” in the lumbar spine through manual palpation. Another interesting
element that is discussed by (Seffinger et al., 2004) is: what
is the clinical relevance of the “fault” ? Only 26% of included studies
recruited only symptomatic subjects and 17% had recruited both symptomatic and
asymptomatic persons. This information leads to a major questioning of the
relevance of what clinicians may find during these types of palpatory
procedures. Furthermore, it is not yet understood how positional faults could
impact pain and range of motion. Some authors proposed models in which the
positional faults could contribute to repetitive stress potentially leading to
pain and / or tissue failure (Comerford & Mottram, 2001; Dye, 2005). Even
though the theory is conceptually attracting, it remains experts’ opinion and low-level
evidence. The third point that tends to be conflicting regarding the
biomechanical fault hypothesis is about the specificity of the intervention
itself. Indeed, the application of a passive physiological or accessory
movement to a precise segmental joint in the lumbar spine may not be supported
by literature (Lee & Evans, 1997). For
example, during a prone posteroanterior mobilization, the force spreads through
a large area moving several joints and all surrounding soft tissues. The
movement affects every joint of the lumbar spine. The whole extension movement
is a three-point bending between the ribcage and the pelvis. A posterior
translation of vertebras superior to the point of applied force occurs while
the inferior joints translate anteriorly (Lee & Evans, 1997). It is
then difficult to admit that a technique that cannot aim a specific area could
correct a “positional fault” which is barely identifiable with extremely
developed imagery. All this again shows that the hypothesis of a biomechanical
fault that would be corrected through manual therapy by the clinician cannot
currently be identified as a fully or partly responsible for SMMT’s effects.
The lack of support of this theory does not imply that
SMM techniques do not have any mechanical
effect. Indeed, human tissues demonstrate viscous and elastic characteristics
and hence react to viscoelastic properties like “creep” and “preconditioning”.
The creep phenomenon is the elongation of a viscoelastic structure subject to a
constant force which is lesser than its rupture point (Twomey & Taylor, 1982). In other
words, while applying a constant strain on a tissue, it will elongate through
time. This phenomenon is probably due to the modification of water distribution
and collagen fibers’ position within the structure (Carlstedt & Nordin, 1989). Creep
increases with load, progresses with time and is age dependent (Twomey & Taylor, 1982).In lumbar spine, sustained loading of a
posteroanterior force at L4 level during 30 seconds showed the greatest amount
of creep and then decreased with time (Evans, 1992). The
preconditioning phenomenon occurs when applying a cyclic loading to a vertebra
leading to an increase of displacement with each loading cycle. The effect of
preconditioning decreases with each cycle (Evans, 1992; Lee & Evans, 1994). Nevertheless,
the effects of creep and preconditioning are reversible, and unloaded
structures tend to return to their original size (Lee & Evans, 1994). Recovery
time has been shown to be longer than creep to occur, but half of the creep
effects disappears within two minutes in lumbar spine of young subjects after
twenty minutes of seated full flexion. (McGill & Brown, 1992). In study
by (Solomonow et al., 2000) after 50
minutes of cyclic loading of cats’ supra-spinal ligament 17% of creep had
recovered after 10 minutes and 92% after 7 hours. In the study by (Lu, Solomonow, Zhou, Baratta, & Li, 2004), after 7
hours rest, 64,4% of creep effect was still present. The duration of creep or
preconditioning cannot be precisely estimated clinically because the studies
mentioned above are laboratory studies, some studied animals’ tissues, and the
holding duration is not representative of the clinical encounter. The point I
am trying to raise is simply that some (temporary) mechanical effect is
produced during mobilization and cannot be denied, however it does not seem
responsible for the efficacy of the techniques alone either.
To summarize this paragraph, it can be said that
transient mechanical effects are likely to happen during mobilization and may
be sufficient to produce immediate increase in range of motion but there is insufficient evidence to
support the positional-fault hypothesis and their correction with SMMT.
Neurophysiological
effects.
With the increasing knowledge in neuroscience and the
difficulties faced when trying to prove the biomechanical effects of manual
therapy; new hypothesis emerged. The neurophysiological effects are the other
main category of effects that could be responsible for the efficacy of manual therapy
techniques. They are mainly subdivided in three areas. The local analgesic
mechanisms, the spinal cord mediated mechanisms and the supra-spinal
mechanisms.
Regarding the local analgesia, evidence is lacking.
The theory is that mobilizations and manipulations could have an
anti-inflammatory effect. This explanation was developed because some studies
showed concurrent to local hypoalgesia measured through pressure pain threshold
: an increase in blood’s substance P level following cervical and dorsal
manipulation (Molina-Ortega et al., 2014); or a
raise in blood’s production of inflammatory cytokines unrelated to substance P
levels following upper thoracic manipulation (Teodorczyk-Injeyan, Ruegg, & Injeyan, 2006) or sympathoexcitation
after MWM. The primary hypoalgesia does not appear linked with endogenous
opioid system (Vicenzino, Paungmali, & Teys, 2007). However,
this theory implies that an inflammatory response is the origin of low back
pain which has not been demonstrated. In addition, the aforementioned studies
were done on asymptomatic subjects and the measures were at a global scale
(blood level) and not locally. It is then difficult to link these observations
with the local hypoalgesic effect. Moreover, we don’t know to what extent this
increase in substance P in the participants’ blood can affect pain or range of
motion in patients. More research is needed to ascertain the involvement of
these local analgesic mechanisms, but literature tends to show their existence
without explaining their impact. In conclusion, local analgesia measured with
pressure pain thresholds exists after mobilizations and manipulations, but we
are unable to understand the mechanisms explaining it at a local level.
The spinal cord-mediated mechanisms are another area
studied that is thought to be involved in effects of SMMT. According to (Pickar
& Bolton 2012) manipulations could stimulate joint’s mechanosensitive
paraspinal primary afferent neurons. They then hypothesized that this primary
afferent discharge could inhibit joint’s nociceptors’ activity. Their reasoning
relies on the “Pain Gate Theory” of (Melzack & Wall 1965) in which mechanoreceptors
could, in dorsal horn of spinal cord, inhibit the afferent activity of
nociceptors. Other studies showed that SMMT could impact temporal summation.
Temporal summation is a phenomenon of increase in pain when a constant
nociceptive stimulus is applied with a precise frequency. It is a phenomenon
known to be occurring in central sensitization and this wind-up phenomenon is happening
at level of spinal horn (J. E. Bialosky, Bishop, Robinson, Zeppieri, &
George, 2009; Bishop, Beneciuk, & George, 2011). In
studies on healthy participants, decrease in temporal summation has been showed
after manipulation at lumbar and thoracic levels (Bishop et al., 2011; George, Bishop, Bialosky,
Zeppieri, & Robinson, 2006). It was
also shown to be reduced after SMMT in low back pain participants (J. E. Bialosky et al., 2009). The
effect is thought to be local, as the measures taken above the manipulated area
had no change in temporal summation. However, it is yet unknown how a short
stimulus could modify efferent pathways for a longer duration lasting after the
end of the mobilization or manipulation. (Boal & Gillette, 2004) studied
the neuronal plasticity of spinal cord’s neurons. They suggested that manual
therapy could provoke long term depression of neurons in dorsal horn leading to
reduced central sensitization. In the other hand, an animal study (Skyba, Radhakrishnan, Rohlwing, Wright, & Sluka,
2003)
in which they blocked specific neurotransmitters showed that the analgesia
obtained after mobilization does not seem affected by dorsal horn mechanisms.
So, it is currently still unclear how this hypothesis of spinal cord mediation
of pain could be responsible for SMMT’s efficacy.
The third theory that has been studied is involving
the supraspinal mechanisms. It is thought that SMMT can stimulate descending
pain inhibitory pathways from periaqueductal grey matter (PAG). The study of (Hosobuchi, Adams, & Linchitz, 1978) showed
that electric stimulation of periventricular and periaqueductal gray matter can
create hypoalgesia in human. This reduction in pain can be inhibited by
naloxone which implies that brain can produce analgesia through morphine-like
substances. (Skyba et al., 2003) showed
that SMMT is likely to stimulate the PAG increasing release of spinal serotonin
and noradrenalin hence hypoalgesia, but they showed that this pathway was
non-opioid mediated. Systematic reviews seems to agree on the fact that SMMT
creates immediate hypoalgesia effect (Lascurain-Aguirrebena, Newham, & Critchley, 2016;
Millan, Leboeuf-Yde, Budgell, & Amorim, 2012; Voogt et al., 2015) at least
on some nociceptive stimuli like pressure pain threshold but not thermal pain
threshold. In contrast, research is conflicting regarding the location of
effects as some authors claim that the effect is global with increased PPT at
remote location (Coronado et al., 2012; Lascurain-Aguirrebena et al.,
2016; Vicenzino, Collins, & Wright, 1996) while other
systematic reviews are less conclusive saying that evidence is conflicting (Millan et al., 2012; Voogt et al., 2015). When
using SMMT, in addition to hypoalgesia, concurrent sympathoexcitation has been
demonstrated (Kingston, Claydon, & Tumilty, 2014) and seem
correlated with it. Indeed, increase in skin conductance and decreased skin
temperature has been measured in a RCT following cervical mobilization on 30
patients (Sterling, Jull, & Wright, 2001). They also
measured decreased motor activity of neck flexor leading to hypothesize that
the effects of manual therapy could be partly due to changes in muscles’ motor
activity. This could be another element that might be a factor in reduction in
pain and increase in patient’s ability to move following SMMT (Zusman, 1986). The model
proposed is that muscle inhibition could concurrently to changes in
nociception, reduce periarticular pressure hence reduction in pain.
To conclude, I would say that currently, precise
supraspinal mechanisms responsible for effects of SMMT are unknown, but some
evidence of its role has clearly been demonstrated and it is currently the most
plausible hypothesis that has been investigated. However, a combination of all
the previous hypothesis cannot be excluded either. The final effect might also
be reached by a combination of many mechanisms from local, spinal-cord and
supraspinal mechanisms.
Non-specific
effects.
The last point I would like to raise is about the nonspecific
effects. As any type of treatment, it generates non-specific effects, so it is
important to mention it. Placebo effect and other effects as patient-clinician
relationship, expectations etc. are present in all type of treatment and cannot
be avoided; so, we should always keep them in mind. In addition, non-specific
effects are the reason why RCT that does not include control group and placebo
like intervention group should be interpreted with caution. Manual contact has
been proven to induce sympathoexcitation measured with increased skin
conductance (Moulson & Watson, 2006; Petersen, Vicenzino,
& Wright, 1993); but also
increased activity of superficial neck flexors when hand contact with neck (Sterling et al., 2001). The
patient’s beliefs about the benefits of the intervention seems linked with its
results. In patients with neck pain, differences were found when comparing
results of an intervention with patient’s expectation about it, showing better
results when the patient had good expectations of relief from the technique (Bishop, Mintken, Bialosky, & Cleland, 2013). Another
study was performed on patients with chronic low back pain and also showed
better outcomes when the patient had good expectations of outcome. It also
showed that the patient’s preference may influence the outcome. Patients
expecting one type of treatment and receiving another one were less likely to
improve than the one receiving the treatment they expected (Kalauokalani, Cherkin, Sherman, Koepsell, & Deyo,
2001).
This is why it is an important part of evidence based practice to assess
patients’ expectations and preferences and discuss treatment options with them (Sackett, Rosenberg, Gray, Haynes, & Richardson,
1996).
The famous, well documented placebo effect also takes part into the global
effect of SSMT. Its precise psychological mechanism of action is not fully
understood but there is evidence of a lot of elements involved (Benedetti, 2005) that
cannot be developed in this blog, as it is not its primary aim. Clinical
outcomes are influenced by all kind of contextual elements ranging from the
patient-physiotherapist’s relationship, the care settings, or personal features
(of patient or therapist) (Testa & Rossettini, 2016). It has
been shown that clinicians can positively or negatively influence outcomes with
verbal suggestions (Van Laarhoven et al., 2011). All this
illustrates well that SMMT’s efficacy can also be influenced positively or
negatively with non-specific effects. So, there is no reason they would not be
a part of SMMT’s efficacy and be reminded in their mechanisms of action.
Conclusion:
Now that I have written this blog, I can eventually
explain what science has proven regarding the effects of spinal mobilization
and manipulation techniques. It has been proven that mechanical effects exist
following SMMT. That neurophysiological effects like hypoalgesia (measured with
increased pressure pain threshold, reduction in VAS scores and functional
scores); sympathoexcitation (measured through skin temperature changes, skin
conductance), and muscles motor activity changes are happening following SMMT.
However, it is not precisely known yet how these parameters are modified at
local, spinal-cord and supraspinal levels although some hypotheses have been
formulated. From now on, every time I will use manual therapy techniques, I
will be able to explain what I expect from it and be aware of current evidence
regarding effects of spinal mobilization and manipulation techniques. This
makes me less of a believer and more of an evidence based driven clinician.
Bialosky, J. E., Bishop, M. D., Robinson, M. E., Zeppieri,
G., & George, S. Z. (2009). Spinal Manipulative Therapy Has an Immediate
Effect on Thermal Pain Sensitivity in People With Low Back Pain: A Randomized
Controlled Trial. Physical Therapy, 89(12), 1292–1303.
https://doi.org/10.2522/ptj.20090058
Bialosky, J. E., George, S. Z., & Bishop, M. D. (2008).
How Spinal Manipulative Therapy Works: Why Ask Why? Journal of Orthopaedic
& Sports Physical Therapy, 38(6), 293–295.
https://doi.org/10.2519/jospt.2008.0118
Bishop, M. D., Beneciuk, J. M., & George, S. Z. (2011).
Immediate reduction in temporal sensory summation after thoracic spinal
manipulation. Spine Journal, 11(5), 440–446.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.spinee.2011.03.001
Bishop, M. D., Mintken, P., Bialosky, J. E., & Cleland,
J. A. (2013). Patient Expectations of Benefit From Interventions for Neck Pain
and Resulting Influence on Outcomes. Journal of Orthopaedic & Sports
Physical Therapy, 43(7), 457–465.
https://doi.org/10.2519/jospt.2013.4492
Boal, R. W., & Gillette, R. G. (2004). Central neuronal plasticity, low back pain and spinal
manipulative therapy. Journal of Manipulative and Physiological Therapeutics.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jmpt.2004.04.005
Carlstedt, C. A., & Nordin, M. (1989). Biomechanics of tendons and ligaments. In Basic
Biomechanics of the Musculoskeletal System.
Comerford, M. J., & Mottram, S. L. (2001). Review
article: Movement and stability dysfunction – Contemporary developments. Manual
Therapy. https://doi.org/10.1054/math.2000.0388
Coronado, R. A., Gay, C. W., Bialosky, J. E., Carnaby, G. D.,
Bishop, M. D., & George, S. Z. (2012). Changes in pain sensitivity
following spinal manipulation: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Journal
of Electromyography and Kinesiology, 22(5), 752–767.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jelekin.2011.12.013
Dye, S. F. (2005). The pathophysiology of patellofemoral
pain: A tissue homeostasis perspective. Clinical Orthopaedics and Related
Research, NA;(436), 100–110.
https://doi.org/10.1097/01.blo.0000172303.74414.7d
Evans, J. (1992). Load-displacement-time characteristics of
the spine under posteroanterior mobilisation. Australian Journal of
Physiotherapy, 38(2), 115–123.
https://doi.org/10.1016/S0004-9514(14)60556-0
George, S. Z., Bishop, M. D., Bialosky, J. E., Zeppieri, G.,
& Robinson, M. E. (2006). Immediate effects of spinal manipulation on
thermal pain sensitivity: An experimental study. BMC Musculoskeletal
Disorders, 7. https://doi.org/10.1186/1471-2474-7-68
Hosobuchi, Y., Adams, J. E., & Linchitz, R. (1978). PAIN
RELIEF BY ELECTRICAL STIMULATION OF THE CENTRAL GRAY MATTER IN HUMANS AND ITS
REVERSAL BY NALOXONE. Survey of Anesthesiology, 22(3), 252???253.
https://doi.org/10.1097/00132586-197806000-00029
Kalauokalani, D., Cherkin, D. C., Sherman, K. J., Koepsell,
T. D., & Deyo, R. A. (2001). Lessons from a trial of acupuncture and
massage for low back pain: Patient expectations and treatment effects. Spine,
26(13), 1418–1424. https://doi.org/10.1097/00007632-200107010-00005
Kingston, L., Claydon, L., & Tumilty, S. (2014). The
effects of spinal mobilizations on the sympathetic nervous system: A systematic
review. Manual Therapy. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.math.2014.04.004
Lascurain-Aguirrebena, I., Newham, D., & Critchley, D. J.
(2016). Mechanism of action of spinal mobilizations a systematic review. Spine,
41(2), 159–172. https://doi.org/10.1097/BRS.0000000000001151
Lee, R., & Evans, J. (1994). Towards a Better Understanding
of Spinal Posteroanterior Mobilisation. Physiotherapy (United Kingdom), 80(2),
68–73. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0031-9406(10)61009-7
Lee, R., & Evans, J. (1997). An in vivo study of the
intervertebral movements produced by posteroanterior mobilization. Clinical
Biomechanics, 12(6), 400–408.
https://doi.org/10.1016/S0268-0033(97)00019-3
Lu, D., Solomonow, M., Zhou, B., Baratta, R. V., & Li, L.
(2004). Frequency-dependent changes in neuromuscular responses to cyclic lumbar
flexion. Journal of Biomechanics, 37(6), 845–855.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jbiomech.2003.11.023
Millan, M., Leboeuf-Yde, C., Budgell, B., & Amorim, M. A.
(2012). The effect of spinal manipulative therapy on experimentally induced
pain: A systematic literature review. Chiropractic and Manual Therapies,
20. https://doi.org/10.1186/2045-709X-20-26
Molina-Ortega, F., Lomas-Vega, R., Hita-Contreras, F., Plaza
Manzano, G., Achalandabaso, A., Ramos-Morcillo, A. J., & Martínez-Amat, A.
(2014). Immediate effects of spinal manipulation on nitric oxide, substance P
and pain perception. Manual Therapy, 19(5), 411–417.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.math.2014.02.007
Moulson, A., & Watson, T. (2006). A preliminary
investigation into the relationship between cervical snags and sympathetic
nervous system activity in the upper limbs of an asymptomatic population. Manual
Therapy, 11(3), 214–224. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.math.2006.04.003
Petersen, N., Vicenzino, B., & Wright, A. (1993). The
effects of a cervical mobilisation technique on sympathetic outflow to the
upper limb in normal subjects. Physiotherapy Theory and Practice, 9(3),
149–156. https://doi.org/10.3109/09593989309047454
Sackett, D. L., Rosenberg, W. M. C., Gray, J. A. M., Haynes,
R. B., & Richardson, W. S. (1996). Evidence based medicine: what it is and
what it isn’t. BMJ, 312(7023), 71–72.
https://doi.org/10.1136/bmj.312.7023.71
Seffinger, M. A., Najm, W. I., Mishra, S. I., Adams, A.,
Dickerson, V. M., Murphy, L. S., & Reinsch, S. (2004). Reliability of
Spinal Palpation for Diagnosis of Back and Neck Pain. Spine, 29(19),
E413–E425. https://doi.org/10.1097/01.brs.0000141178.98157.8e
Skyba, D. A., Radhakrishnan, R., Rohlwing, J. J., Wright, A.,
& Sluka, K. A. (2003). Joint manipulation reduces hyperalgesia by
activation of monoamine receptors but not opioid or GABA receptors in the
spinal cord. Pain, 106(1–2), 159–168.
https://doi.org/10.1016/S0304-3959(03)00320-8
Solomonow, M., He Zhou, B., Baratta, R. V., Lu, Y., Zhu, M.,
& Harris, M. (2000). Biexponential recovery model of lumbar viscoelastic
laxity and reflexive muscular activity after prolonged cyclic loading. Clinical
Biomechanics, 15(3), 167–175. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0268-0033(99)00062-5
Sterling, M., Jull, G., & Wright, A. (2001). Cervical
mobilisation: Concurrent effects on pain, sympathetic nervous system activity
and motor activity. Manual Therapy, 6(2), 72–81.
https://doi.org/10.1054/math.2000.0378
Teodorczyk-Injeyan, J. A., Ruegg, R., & Injeyan, H. S.
(2006). Spinal manipulative therapy reduces inflammatory cytokines but not
substance P production in normal subjects. In Journal of Manipulative and
Physiological Therapeutics (Vol. 29, pp. 14–21). https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jmpt.2005.10.002
Testa, M., & Rossettini, G. (2016). Enhance placebo,
avoid nocebo: How contextual factors affect physiotherapy outcomes. Manual
Therapy, 24, 65–74. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.math.2016.04.006
Van Laarhoven, A. I. M., Vogelaar, M. L., Wilder-Smith, O.
H., Van Riel, P. L. C. M., Van De Kerkhof, P. C. M., Kraaimaat, F. W., &
Evers, A. W. M. (2011). Induction of nocebo and placebo
effects on itch and pain by verbal suggestions. Pain, 152(7),
1486–1494. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pain.2011.01.043
Vicenzino, B., Collins, D., & Wright, A. (1996). The
initial effects of a cervical spine manipulative physiotherapy treatment on the
pain and dysfunction of lateral epicondylalgia. Pain, 68(1),
69–74. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0304-3959(96)03221-6
Vicenzino, B., Paungmali, A., & Teys, P. (2007).
Mulligan’s mobilization-with-movement, positional faults and pain relief:
Current concepts from a critical review of literature. Manual Therapy.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.math.2006.07.012
Voogt, L., de Vries, J., Meeus, M., Struyf, F., Meuffels, D.,
& Nijs, J. (2015). Analgesic effects of manual therapy in patients with
musculoskeletal pain: A systematic review. Manual Therapy, 20(2),
250–256. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.math.2014.09.001
Zusman, M. (1986). Spinal Manipulative Therapy: Review of
Some Proposed Mechanisms, and a New Hypothesis. Australian Journal of
Physiotherapy, 32(2), 89–99.
https://doi.org/10.1016/S0004-9514(14)60645-0
Questions:
Hi Clement,
An interesting read. I just have a couple of questions and I will try to keep them short! You start talking about the accuracy of using palpation as a diagnostic tool to identify the structure at fault. You ask “How can I know that the ‘restriction’ noticed during my assessment is related to the patient’s presentation if it can be found in asymptomatic persons?” I would like to ask you isn’t this a similar issue to a lot of diagnostic testing in the physiotherapy world? Therefore, do you think you can still use this along with other subjective/objective information to support your hypothesis? If so, could you expand on how you would do this and what other information you could use to support your clinical rationale? Finally, I would ask how relevant is knowing the exact structure at fault in relation to getting the patient better and do you know of any evidence looking at this?
There are a few questions there which roll into one and hopefully you get my point.
Best wishes,
Mellissa
Thank you for your questions
Melissa.
Regarding the first part of
your first question about the use of palpation: I completely agree with the
fact that this issue is common in our practice. But knowing this does not give
any justification for or against the use of palpation as a diagnostic tool. I
would like to specify that I do not condemn the use of palpation for diagnostic
purpose. My aim was to inform that alone, palpation of a hypothetical
“positional fault” is not enough to link it with the patient’s issue, as these
“faults” can be found in asymptomatic population. Therefore, it cannot be used
to validate the hypothesis of positional faults to explain the efficacy of
SMMT.
However, as you mentioned in the second part of
your question, if these findings are linked with subjective assessment or
symptom reproduction; then they might be useful for the clinician. (Rabey et
al., 2017) Not that they would indicate a positional fault, but they could
indicate a local tenderness to mobilisation or pressure that could be linked
with other elements of the assessment. They could be used as an asterisk marker
for the clinician and reassessed after SSMT treatment for example. So, I could
use palpation in combination with symptom reproduction, limited range of
motion, elements from the subjective assessment to get a global picture of the
patient. But it would still not provide any information to support a positional
misalignment of a vertebral segment. Indeed, there is no evidence of this
theory as I explained it in the paragraph. To conclude, my point was not to
blame the use of palpation in clinical practice as it provides information; it
simply was to illustrate that it cannot explain in itself the theory proposed
to explain the “positional fault hypothesis” which still has not been
demonstrated.
Then to answer your second question about
knowing the exact structure at fault, it is a really interesting topic even
though it is more related to diagnosis of LBP and efficacy of SSMT than their
effects. However, the assumption according to which knowing which structure is
responsible for the patient’s issue is attractive. Especially when trying to
figure out what is happening during the technique we perform. However, we saw
in the blog that we cannot reliably identify the restricted level with
palpation nor with imagery. Let’s imagine we could, would it be more efficient
than applying SMMT randomly on the spine? Some articles seem to lead in this
direction. (Aquino et al., 2009) showed in the cervical spine that improvement
in pain is achieved irrespective of the location of the mobilisation in one
session in a chronic cervical pain sample of patient. Regarding this study,
several elements should be considered. The sample is relatively small. The
random location is not developed and could be close enough to the most
restricted segment to mobilise it too, especially in such a small area as
cervical spine. As it has been shown in lumbar spine, mobilisation do not
affect a precise joint but multiple joints around the point of application (Lee
& Evans, 1997). Aquino’s study measured only pain response and did not
evaluate mobility, and it was measured in chronic patients on the results of
only one session of treatment without developing how long and how many
repetitions were used. Then, it is difficult to draw firm conclusions against
the use of targeted mobilisation. Another study also seems to suggest that
directing a manipulation technique towards a precise or random location has the
same effects. This study should also be interpreted carefully as the specific
treatment used is not specific. Indeed, manipulations have been showed to occur
in the whole thoracolumbar spine but that they could not be aimed at a
segmental level (Bialosky, George, & Bishop, 2008). So, this study compares
two non-specific techniques. Then it cannot be concluded that all SMMT can
randomly be applied to produce effects. The third study is the most interesting
and best conducted one. A study by (Donaldson, Petersen, Cook, & Learman,
2016), showed that lumbar segmental mobilisation in chronic low back pain
patients do not differ whether it is performed on the most provocative level or
on L4-5. One issue here is that the most painful level has not been assessed in
patients receiving L4-5 mobilisation and a part of this group could have their
tender spot on this location. The study showed no difference between groups.
But it can be due to the absence of selection of patients that might be better
responders to this type of treatment. Maybe if correctly identifying responders
to SSMT, targeting the mobilisation on this subgroup of patient would be more
beneficial; but this is still an under-studied area (Rabey et al., 2017) and it
does not refute the study’s results in anyway. However, interestingly even
though the study showed no difference in outcomes between groups, it still
revealed a significantly better global rate of change (GROC) in the targeted
mobilisation group at the 4th session and six-month follow-up. This result
indicate that a difference might be achieved in some way. The GROC might be a
tool which is global, it is still interesting. Indeed, as will be developed in
part two and three and already mentioned in the previous part, SMMT needs to be
viewed as a tool to improve the patient as a whole. Within a new framework in
which through SMMT we can modify the patient’s symptom, and in combination with
explanation about pain, bringing confidence in movement and empowering the
patient to move; SMMT may still have an important role to play in
rehabilitation.
To conclude I would say that these techniques
have effects on the person for sure, but its mechanisms of action are more
global than explained by “positional fault” theory initially introduced in
1989. However, even though SMMT cannot affect a precise vertebra, it is worth
considering the location of maximal stiffness and symptoms while applying them.
First because it is more likely to fit with the patient’s expectation or belief
and hence increase non-specific effects (see part 3). Secondly, because we are
not sure yet whether it can improve outcomes or not. If targeting a spinal
level may or not improve outcome for some patients, it surely does not worsen
results either. So, it is worth applying them on the most provocative location
rather assuming that it does not make a difference. It does not require much
more effort from the clinician to apply pressure on a spot or another, but it
might lead to better outcomes. Then, I would definitely go for the most
provocative one.
References:
Aquino, R., Caires, P., Furtado, F., Loureiro,
A., Ferreira, P., & Ferreira, M. (2009). Applying Joint mobilization at
Different Cervical vertebral Levels does not Influence Immediate Pain Reduction
in Patients with Chronic Neck Pain: A Randomized Clinical Trial. The Journal of
Manual & Manipulative Therapy, 17(2), 95–100. https://doi.org/DOI:
10.1179/106698109790824686
Bialosky, J. E., George, S. Z., & Bishop, M. D.
(2008). How Spinal Manipulative Therapy Works: Why Ask Why? Journal of
Orthopaedic & Sports Physical Therapy, 38(6), 293–295.
https://doi.org/10.2519/jospt.2008.0118
Donaldson, M., Petersen, S., Cook, C., &
Learman, K. (2016). A Prescriptively Selected Nonthrust Manipulation Versus a
Therapist-Selected Nonthrust Manipulation for Treatment of Individuals With Low
Back Pain: A Randomized Clinical Trial. Journal of Orthopaedic & Sports
Physical Therapy, 46(4), 243–250. https://doi.org/10.2519/jospt.2016.6318
Lee, R., & Evans, J. (1997). An in vivo study
of the intervertebral movements produced by posteroanterior mobilization.
Clinical Biomechanics, 12(6), 400–408.
https://doi.org/10.1016/S0268-0033(97)00019-3
Rabey, M., Hall, T., Hebron, C., Palsson, T. S., Christensen, S. W., & Moloney, N. (2017). Reconceptualising manual therapy skills in contemporary practice. Musculoskeletal Science and Practice, 29, 28–32. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.msksp.2017.02.010